Rating: ****
Tags: Self-Help, Lang:en
Publisher: Penguin Group USA
Added: March 30, 2018
Modified: March 28, 2020
Summary
Moonwalking with Einstein follows Joshua Foer's
compelling journey as a participant in the U.S. Memory
Championship. As a science journalist covering the
competition, Foer became captivated by the secrets of the
competitors, like how the current world memory champion, Ben
Pridmore, could memorize the exact order of 1,528 digits in
an hour. He met with individuals whose memories are truly
unique—from one man whose memory only extends back to
his most recent thought, to another who can memorize complex
mathematical formulas without knowing any math. Brains
remember visual imagery but have a harder time with other
information, like lists, and so with the help of experts,
Foer learned how to transform the kinds of memories he forgot
into the kind his brain remembered naturally. The techniques
he mastered made it easier to remember information, and
Foer's story demonstrates that the tricks of the masters are
accessible to anyone.
Author Q&A with Joshua Foer
Q: First, can you explain the title of you
book,
Moonwalking with Einstein?
A: The title refers to a memory device I
used in the US Memory Championship—specifically it's a
mnemonic that helped me memorize a deck of playing cards.
Moonwalking with Einstein works as a mnemonic because it's
such a goofy image. Things that are weird or colorful are the
most memorable. If you try to picture Albert Einstein sliding
backwards across a dance floor wearing penny loafers and a
diamond glove, that's pretty much unforgettable.
Q: What are the U.S. Memory Championships?
How did you become involved?
A: The U.S. Memory Championship is a rather
bizarre contest held each spring in New York City, in which
people get together to see who can remember the most names of
strangers, the most lines of poetry, the most random digits.
I went to the event as a science journalist, to cover what I
assumed would be the Super Bowl of savants. But when I talked
to the competitors, they told me something really
interesting. They weren't savants. And they didn't have
photographic memories. Rather, they'd trained their memories
using ancient techniques. They said anyone could do it. I was
skeptical. Frankly, I didn't believe them. I said, well, if
anyone can do it, could you teach me? A guy named Ed Cooke,
who has one of the best trained memories in the world, took
me under his wing and taught me everything he knew about
memory techniques. A year later I came back to the contest,
this time to try and compete, as a sort of exercise in
participatory journalism. I was curious simply to see how
well I'd do, but I ended up winning the contest. That really
wasn't supposed to happen.
Q: What was the most surprising thing you
found out about yourself competing in the Memory
Championships?
A: In the process of studying these
techniques, I learned something remarkable: that there's far
more potential in our minds than we often give them credit
for. I'm not just talking about the fact that it's possible
to memorize lots of information using memory techniques. I'm
talking about a lesson that is more general, and in a way
much bigger: that it's possible, with training and hard work,
to teach oneself to do something that might seem really
difficult.
Q: Can you explain the "OK Plateau?"
A: The OK Plateau is that place we all get
to where we just stop getting better at something. Take
typing, for example. You might type and type and type all day
long, but once you reach a certain level, you just never get
appreciably faster at it. That's because it's become
automatic. You've moved it to the back of your mind's filing
cabinet. If you want to become a faster typer, it's possible,
of course. But you've got to bring the task back under your
conscious control. You've got to push yourself past where
you're comfortable. You have to watch yourself fail and learn
from your mistakes. That's the way to get better at anything.
And it's how I improved my memory.
Q: What do you mean by saying there an "art"
to memory?
A: The "art of memory" refers to a set of
techniques that were invented in ancient Greece. These are
the same techniques that Cicero used to memorize his
speeches, and that medieval scholars used to memorize entire
books. The "art" is in creating imagery in your mind that is
so unusual, so colorful, so unlike anything you've ever seen
before that it's unlikely to be forgotten. That's why
mnemonists like to say that their skills are as much about
creativity as memory.
Q: How do you think technology has affected
how and what we remember?
A: Once upon a time people invested in their
memories, they cultivated them. They studiously furnished
their minds. They remembered. Today, of course, we've got
books, and computers and smart phones to hold our memories
for us. We've outsourced our memories to external devices.
The result is that we no longer trust our memories. We see
every small forgotten thing as evidence that they're failing
us altogether. We've forgotten how to remember.
Q: What is the connection between memory and
our sense of time?
A: As we get older, life seems to fly by
faster and faster. That's because we structure our experience
of time around memories. We remember events in relation to
other events. But as we get older, and our experiences become
less unique, our memories can blend together. If yesterday's
lunch is indistinguishable from the one you ate the day
before, it'll end up being forgotten. That's why it's so hard
to remember meals. In the same way, if you're not doing
things that are unique and different and memorable, this year
can come to resemble the last, and end up being just as
forgettable as yesterday's lunch. That's why it's so
important to pack your life with interesting experiences that
make your life memorable, and provide a texture to the
passage of time.
Q: How is your memory now?
A: Ironically, not much better than when I
started this whole journey. The techniques I learned, and
used in the memory contest, are great for remembering
structured information like shopping lists or phone numbers,
but they don't improve any sort of underlying, generalizable
memory ability. Unfortunately, I still misplace my car
keys.
(Photo of Joshua Foer © Emil Salman Haaretz) If you sometimes can�t remember where you put your
car keys or, like Foer, the car itself, don�t panic.
You�re not alone, and you can do something about it.
In this intriguing look at the nature of memory, Foer
reassures us that we don�t need to acquire a better
memory; we just need to use the one we have more effectively.
Foer introduces us to people whose memories are both
astonishing, like the man who could memorize 1,528 random
digits in order, and frightening, such as a man with such an
extreme case of amnesia that he doesn�t know his own
age and can�t remember that he has a memory problem.
He explores various ways in which we test our memories, such
as the extensive training British cabbies must undergo. He
also discusses ways we can train ourselves to have better
memories, like the PAO system, in which, for example, every
card in a deck is associated with an image of a specific
person, action, or object. An engaging, informative, and for
the forgetful, encouraging book. --David PittAmazon.com Review
--Miriam Landis
From